Hit children?
5 posters
Page 1 of 1
Re: Hit children?
This was a major beating. I counted 10 bloody welts in the one picture I saw on tmz.com.
Guest- Guest
Re: Hit children?
The one positive..it stirred up a national debate on the issue of hitting children..long overdue...
Re: Hit children?
It's shocking to think that it would be such a huge issue given the amount of research that proves it can cause PTSD in children.
Guest- Guest
Re: Hit children?
Here is one, skip ahead to the spanking section
[You must be registered and logged in to see this link.]
[You must be registered and logged in to see this link.]
Guest- Guest
Re: Hit children?
A good old fashioned, open-hand spanking on the butt. Yes, I believe in old fashion discipline. There's a difference between spanking your kid in the way I mentioned and "hitting kids".
Re: Hit children?
Study after study has shown spanking to be ineffective in the long term at best and most likely harmful at worst
Hitting children just teaches them that might makes right and that it's okay for bigger people to hit smaller people. It teaches that problems should be solved through violence.
I realize this is going against the grain, but my research into this has led to this position.
Hitting children just teaches them that might makes right and that it's okay for bigger people to hit smaller people. It teaches that problems should be solved through violence.
I realize this is going against the grain, but my research into this has led to this position.
Re: Hit children?
Beating children senseless, and of that nature is wrong. A good old fashion ass whooping is what most parents need to start doing. I honestly believe I came out the way I am because I never received a good ass whooping.
Forum Gawd- Boss
- Number of posts : 4842
Registration date : 2009-09-16
Age : 30
Location : Athol Street Nd Hemlock.!!!
Re: Hit children?
Parents been giving kids " good old fashion" ass whoopings for centuries and look where we are today.
Crime, drug addiction, mental health problems, and lowered IQ's correlate more with people who were hit than those who weren't..
I've read that up to 80 percent of prison inmates were hit or raised in violent homes. There seems to be no creditable evidence for the common assertion that spanking and whooping children prevents crime or misbehavior. There is evidence that you can stop a small child from doing some unwanted behavior in the short term by physically overpowering them(not hard) but that's a short term solution. It doesn't teach them negotiation skills and how to work with others in a peaceful manner.
Crime, drug addiction, mental health problems, and lowered IQ's correlate more with people who were hit than those who weren't..
I've read that up to 80 percent of prison inmates were hit or raised in violent homes. There seems to be no creditable evidence for the common assertion that spanking and whooping children prevents crime or misbehavior. There is evidence that you can stop a small child from doing some unwanted behavior in the short term by physically overpowering them(not hard) but that's a short term solution. It doesn't teach them negotiation skills and how to work with others in a peaceful manner.
Re: Hit children?
[size=18]Growing Up With Violence[/size]
Growing up in a violent home is one of the most terrifying and traumatic experiences a child can go through.
It’s an experience that a child will not forget. It’s an experience that can affect every aspect of a child’s life, growth and their development.
Living in a violent home can cause children not to do well in school.
There is a definite correlation between domestic violence and child abuse. Growing up in a violent home can set patterns for children … patterns that can cause them to commit violence and abuse, and continue the cycle of violence and abuse.
Children living in violent homes are often too frightened and embarrassed to speak out.
Kids who grow up in violent environments are more apt to have health problems, use poor judgment, and have social and emotional issues.
They are also more apt to become high school dropouts, substance abusers, pregnant teens, gun users, and become juvenile and adult criminals.
Children who suffer from abuse can become adults with a host of additional problems such as poor self-esteem, staying in dead-end jobs or worse … not being able to keep a job.
There are high unemployment rates among adult abused children. They can be full of anger, can mistrust in relationships, are more apt to be bullies, commit road rage, and more horrific violent acts, and contribute to the high cost of our mental health and welfare programs.
We know that domestic violence causes serious harm to the millions of women who are abused, yet too little attention has been paid to the harm suffered by their children who witness domestic abuse.
Kids who grow up in violent homes can be helped through Intervention, prevention, and support programs. But there is much more to be done to keep them safe.
We must educate the public, advocate for more arrests, and stronger punishments for offenders.
America’s children who grow up in violent homes are forgotten. By creating awareness and educating the public, we can promote community and social responsibility – we can stop violence and help abused children.
It’s reported that 50 percent of the men who frequently assault their wives, also frequently abuse their children. School-age kids who grow up in violent homes generally exhibit a range of problem behaviors such as: depression, anxiety, and violence towards their peers.
When a parent terrorizes another parent, their children are terrorized too! Anger is deeply set within those children … anger that is so deep and long-lasting that when that child reaches adulthood, the damage is already done.
The terrorist parent leaves his children an incredible legacy of pain and problems -- societal and emotional problems that may never disappear.
An anonymous victim of domestic violence said:
She didn’t think she ever could have known, what an impact and witnessing domestic violence would have on her son’s life. Her son witnessed domestic violence continuously for the first six years of his life. His behavior became progressively worse, especially as he began to socialize with other kids. In kindergarten he became outraged if something did not go his way, and on many occasions, bit other children so hard that he drew blood. He would often slam and break things for no apparent reason, and had constant violent temper tantrums. It was at this time that this victim found the courage to leave the violent relationship she had with her son’s father. However as the years progressed, her son’s behavior escalated. He was physically aggressive towards other students, and constantly walked out of class when teachers confronted him with his disruptive behavior. The school alerted the mother to the fact that her son’s behavior wasn’t normal. Although her struggle as a victim is over, she must now stand by and watch her son struggle through the same journey. She feels she failed to protect him. There is nothing she can do to make it better, as her son experiences the effects of a family legacy that has claimed many victims. She has now sought psychological assistance for her son to deal with this long and painful legacy.
Violence and abuse have become our country’s legacy. When a parent hurts their child – physically or emotionally, we all feel the affects.
We know that violence is learned behavior. Children learn it from their parents and the cycle continues.
We also know that whatever is learned can be unlearned. It is up to all of us to be educated and learn all of the signs, symptoms and what we can do to stop children from hurting – to destroy the legacy!
CHILDREN WHO GROW UP IN HOMES WHERE VIOLENCE IS PRESENT ARE:
- 6 times more likely to commit suicide
- 24 times more likely to be sexually assaulted
- 67 times more likely to engage in delinquent behavior as an adolescent
- 100 times more likely to be abusers themselves
- 1500 times more likely to be abused or neglected
United States Department of Justice: Special Report, 2002
[You must be registered and logged in to see this link.]
Guest- Guest
Re: Hit children?
[ltr]The scientific evidence against spanking, timeouts, and sleep training[/ltr]
[ltr]Written by
Roger ThompsonAssociate professor, Stony Brook University
December 18, 2014[/ltr]
We're really messing with their brains. (AP Photo)
[ltr]At the end of a gravel road in the Chippewa National Forest of northern Minnesota, a group of camp counselors has gathered to hear psychotherapist Tina Bryson speak about neuroscience, mentorship, and camping. She is in Minnesota by invitation of the camp. Chippewa is at the front of a movement to bring brain science to bear on the camping industry; she keynoted this past year’s American Camping Association annual conference. As Bryson speaks to the counselors gathered for training, she emphasizes one core message: at the heart of effective discipline is curiosity—curiosity on the part of the counselors to genuinely understand and respect the campers’ experience while away from home.
Brain science is far from a precise field, but Bryson deploys it effectively when she conducts trainings. She has lectured from Australia to Germany, California to DC, and the camp trainings are only a small portion of what she does. She envisions herself as on a mission to change parenting, and her talks weave recent data on brain imaging, new findings from top journals, and reports of ongoing experimental research with stories from her own life, anecdotes from her clinical practice, and aw-shucks pithy sayings that help make the science accessible.
Bryson is not alone in this approach. She is part of a progressive new group of scientists, doctors, and psychologists whose goal is ambitious, if not outright audacious: they want to redefine “discipline” in order to change our culture. They want to rewrite, or perhaps more precisely said, rewire how we approach interacting with kids, and they want us to understand that our decisions about parenting affect not only our children’s minds, but ours as well.
So, we’re going to need to toss out our old discipline mainstays. Say goodbye to timeouts. So long spanking and other ritualized whacks. And cry-it-out sleep routines? Mercifully, they too can be a thing of the past. And yet, we can still help our children mature and grow. In fact, people like Bryson think we’ll do it better. If we are going to take seriously what science tells us about how we form relationships and how our mind develops, we will need to construct new strategies for parenting, and when we do, says this new group of researchers, we just may change the world.
In the early 1990s, Rizzolatti and his team had been conducting a series of experiments involving the monkeys and peanuts. A portion of the monkeys’ skulls had been removed, and the scientists had connected a series of wires directly to the brain. The wires conveyed electrical currents from the brain to sophisticated recording devices that could read the neurological patterns of the monkey’s actions and, presumably, its thinking. The experiment proceeded as expected until one day one of Rizollati’s team members noticed something unusual. When a monkey witnessed a researcher reaching for a peanut, the neurons in the monkey’s brain fired in precisely the same way that they did when the monkey itself reached for a peanut. In other words, as far as the brain was concerned, simply seeing someone grab a peanut is the same as actually grabbing it oneself.
The group immediately perceived the importance of the discovery. If seeing was, in terms of the brain, the same as doing, the entire canon of human behavior would have to be rewritten. When Rizolatti attempted to publish their findings, however, the first journal purportedly rejected the article because it found the results to be too inconsequential to consider, even if it did find them credible and accurate. A second journal, though, published details of the experiment, and several years later, another journal published a follow up set of findings. In that paper, Rizzolatti and his team coined a term for the neurological phenomenon they were witnessing: mirror neurons. The term would make him famous.
Since that time, hundreds if not thousands of articles have been published on mirror neurons. They have been credited with generating empathy in humans, fostering love between people, and providing new hope in the research on autism. Yet, the term and the idea of “mirror neurons” continue to prompt considerable controversy. Some researchers argue that empirical evidence for the existence of any neurons that function as “mirrors” is scant, while others suggest that neuroscience has yet to fully grasp the implications of neurons behaving in this way. Regardless of the final direction of these debates, the discussion about mirror neurons has pressed neuroscience into new frontiers, and it has suggested new avenues of inquiry for not only scientists, but doctors and psychologists. Among those avenues is a relatively recent field of study called interpersonal neurobiology. While mirror neurons are not the explicit foundation of the new field, the growth of it is virtually unimaginable without the discoveries by Rizzolatti and his Parma team.
Dan Siegel is the forefather of this movement, coining the term “interpersonal neurobiology” in the late 1990s and effectively launching the new discipline. His colleague Allan Schore, a neuropsychologist who now edits a Norton book series in the field, shares credit as co-founder of the field, but Siegel’s work has transitioned out of the research lab and into the popular press, and he has become an increasingly visible figure in the press circuit. A professor at UCLA’s School of Medicine and author of a flurry of bestsellers, Siegel has sent dozens of followers into the field over the last decade. Some, like Julia Wright and Tina Bryson, are finding their own footing as international experts and sought-after speakers, and they reflect the expanding influence of interpersonal neurobiology as a way of thinking about family relationships. Wright’s forthcoming Penguin release, Happy Sleeper (with Heather Turgeon), suggests new approaches to childhood sleep routines, and Bryson, Siegel’s co-author on the New York Times bestsellers The Whole-Brain Child and No Drama Discipline, redefines discipline more broadly in an attempt to encourage parents to reconsider their approach to child development.
Siegel remains a sought-after speaker in his own right, and the ideas emerging from his Mindful Awareness Research Center at UCLA are finding their ways into not only parenting networks and the popular press, but schools across the country. Some, like the Blue School in New York City, even shape their entire curriculum around his ideas.
Siegel received his medical degree from Harvard, and his contributions to science come in large part from his creation of the new, interdisciplinary field. Interpersonal neurobiology envisions the brain as a social organ, one whose processes can best be understood by its interaction with a variety of complex systems. Among those systems, and one that sets the field apart from simply neurology or biology, is the emotional system that develops in relationships. In terms of what happens inside the brain, that system is just as real as, say, the system that processes our eyesight and deciphers it into meaningful images for us.
Siegel, Bryson, and others in the field simplify the science behind their work by relying on the longstanding theory of the “triune brain,” a theory that divides the brain into three general parts: the brain stem, the limbic system, and the cerebral cortex. The cerebral cortex is our rational, human brain, and the brain stem is, in the words of many scientists, our “reptilian” or lizard brain, the one that basically exists to keep us alive in times of threat. The limbic system functions as a connector, but remains primitive, often encouraging behaviors without giving the cortex time to process and encourage a different, frequently better course of action. When we discipline, argue Siegel and Bryson, we often meet reactive, emotional limbic with limbic, or worse, the lizard brains take control in not just our child, who is raising hell and biting and hissing like a pissed off gecko, but in us as well, as we raise our voices and flail about trying to scare off the lizard by transforming into a bigger, meaner one. Will the Komodo dragon beat the gecko? In some ways, sure. But that little lizard learns one thing, and that is for it to win, it needs to grow stronger, get bigger, and bite harder. If, however, parents can channel their inner Steve Irwins and find ways to approach the lizard child with respect for how it is acting—which is ultimately in an adaptive and useful way to keep it alive in the face of danger and stress—then we might not only make contact with the creature, but teach it that it has nothing to fear so that it can back away, return to its cave, and let the less hissy, more rational kid come back out to play.
Our brain nurtures interaction with others through an almost impossibly complicated set of activities that can nonetheless be observed, documented, and, to some extent, measured. Relationships forge and develop neurons and neural pathways, which means that close observation of what actually occurs neurologically in the brain can help us understand what kinds of relationships work well and which do not. Equally important, the “brain” extends well beyond the organ within our skull. Nerves spread throughout the body, and those nerves are part of a larger body-wide brain that, for lack of a better term, wires together our thinking, our feelings, and our senses. They ultimately, as well, help us make sense of those very systems because we are capable of turning inward and becoming to some degree aware of the processes that make them work. This turning inward to observe and intentionally notice our own body and brain processes Siegel calls “mindsight,” and it allows individuals to make sense of their experiences and consciously integrate their various systems. Just as our five senses collect information that we use to discern the world around us, our mindsight can provide us with clearer views of our own internal world so that we can better relate to the people around us. When we see the mind as an intersecting web of systems whose processes, even if not fully understood, can be illuminated, then we have an opportunity to delve deeper into the significance of our relationships.
Much of what Siegel wants us to consider can be condensed into a simple phrase: “what fires together, wires together.” The idea is that when a set of neurons are stimulated, they link up with all those other neurons that are simultaneously firing. Whether the groups of neurons that are linking make sense to us as observers on the outside is beside the point. Odd pairings can occur, strange juxtapositions of feelings and sensations that, outside of the experience of a particular individual, seem almost impossible to the rest of us. I’m reminded of a narrative in the old DSM-IV casebook that describes an individual who had come to associate sexual arousal with being covered in insects. As a child, that individual had been locked into closets for unimaginable amounts of time, and during those times, bugs would frequently fill the space and crawl on him. The child, trying to seek some sort of escape from the reality of his experience, found comfort only in sexual release, even though he was too young to even know what sex was or meant. His body knew only that it felt good, and it provided the only possible escape available to him. It soothed in the midst of trauma. Those associations—comfort through sex and the sheer, incomprehensible horror, fear, and rage at being locked away in a closet full of insects—became in that mind, quite literally, wired together, so that sex, horror, pleasure, rage, and insects, became bundled as a mass of neurons that shared the same communication pathway. Siegel wants us to become aware of those types of associations and, just as importantly, types that are more mundane and quotidian. They are as much physical as they are “mental,” and they can be anything from unsurprising to astonishing. Sexual gratification and bugs, it turns out, can go together, despite what most of us imagine.
Despite their potential horror, these surprising pairings illustrate the complexity of the brain, and they suggest that if problematic associations can be wired together, so too can more uplifting ones. They point to the possibility that we can forge positive associations in order to create more meaningful, more encouraging, and more beautiful change when we discipline our children. We have known, though not always why, that if we can help a child associate certain behaviors and ways of being with positive stimuli, the child will likely want to replicate the behavior. Siegel and his followers argue that we will be less effective in helping children grow until we understand that children’s actions often have odd pairings at the heart of them and our role is to help children makes sense of them and, with any luck, shift them.
Today, the classroom remains the center of much debate about discipline, but it will likely be in the home that Siegel’s and Bryson’s ideas are put to the test. Their discussions of what happens in the brain when children are being “disciplined” in various ways conflict directly with certain of our parenting practices. The most telling point of conflict is one of the vexing questions of parenting that lingers from generation to generation: to spank or not to spank.
Among some quarters of the American populace, spanking is tantamount to abuse, yet in others, it remains a required part child rearing. The division between the two camps is apparent, as some researchers have suggested, along racial and, probably more tellingly, socio-economic lines. Recent discussions in response to allegations of abuse against Adrian Peterson bear witness to those lines—opportunistic punditry from the likes of Charles Barkley and Steve Harvey have connected the act of spanking to the cultural and economic realities of the black community.
Still, the division can also be drawn, if imperfectly, geographically. In the South, a growing movement over the last two decades has returned the rod to parenting. The movement, often couched in terms of “parental rights,” seeks to ensure that parents have the right to use corporeal punishment with their children. The difference is that the new spank world is not the swack-in-an-instant belting of our parents’ generation. This one is based on the idea that genuine love can be conveyed with a whack on the tush, and that children, because of their little lizard brains, understand and respond to physical stimuli better than verbal.
In fact, they are at least in part right. The animal brain of the child is quite sensitive to touch of all sorts. It recognizes the safety of a hug as well as the danger of a slap without the slightest bit of explanation, and it learns rather quickly that certain behaviors can lead to danger and a red ass. The problem, for folks like Siegel and Bryson, is that children enter a world of emotional chaos when their attachment figure, from whom they are wired to seek safety and security, becomes the figure who also inflicts physical harm. The animal brain, the one that seeks fight or flight, is at that moment conflicted, confused, and, probably downright pissed off. As the brain stem and limbic system instinctually tells the child that danger is coming and that he needs to seek safety and security in the embrace of the attachment figure, the limbic system also confronts the reality that the attachment figure is, in fact, the source of danger. Safety and danger conflated. Brain chemistry roundly fucked up. You might picture Curly running around in circles looking for a place to find safety from Moe, who pops him repeatedly on the forehead. Like poor Curly, lizard brain has very few ways to decipher what is happening and so just circles round and round, occasionally slapping anything or anyone (presumably Larry) nearby.
This internal conflict can lead to what neuropsychologists call “dysregulation.” The neurons start forging relationships that don’t make sense to the more advanced parts of our brain, and as the mind tries to integrate the information, it seeks out solutions, associations, and meaning. When that meaning is hard to discern, as in the case of spanking or even the threat of spanking, the child brain becomes increasingly frustrated, and it essentially dis-integrates. Melt-down ensues. More importantly, it is difficult to construct a coherent meaningful lesson and skills have not been built to make the child more adaptive next time. It has gained no new ways to interpret information, nor has it gained any new ways of making sense of the world with the cortex part of the brain. Instead, the brain-stem and the limbic area stay in charge, and the child, unable to process the conflict, learns a temporary, if also momentarily effective, lesson: if I do X, dad whips my tail. This is not insight or learning or skill-building, this is lizard logic.
Decades of research into spanking demonstrates what happens in the brain when we hit a child, even though it’s only been recently that we’ve been able to make sense of some of the findings. Children who are spanked are, according to a preponderance of the studies, more likely to commit crimes, more likely to suffer from depression, more likely to go to jail, more likely to get into fights, more likely to commit suicide, and more likely to abuse alcohol and drugs. They also typically have lower IQs and poorer academic performance. The studies are, of course, mixed, and they suggest correlations, not causations. We might expect, for instance, that a child with other learning issues may also have more behavioral issues, and so connecting the two doesn’t necessarily point to spanking as a culprit as much as some underlying issue with a particular child or a community. Still, a mountain of evidence suggests that spanking does very little good, if any at all. Most of this we know when we care to slow down and remember our own responses to spankings. Ask yourself, when you were spanked, did you absolutely, never again take an extra cookie from the cookie jar? In all likelihood, you probably just developed ninja-like stealth. Further, the research suggests that spanking may be, and I offer this tidbit without comment, more likely to be connected to political orientation than other traits. Spankers and spankees trend toward Republicanism. That last bit—political conservatives being and spawning spankers—points to why changing methods of discipline is so difficult and why science faces an uphill battle in facilitating change. Upending years of habits is hard enough with family politics, but when the issue becomes entrenched in national politics, it becomes even more difficult.
Discipline has always been politicized. The nineteenth-century debates that focused on whether or not corporeal punishment was appropriate for children gave way in the mid-twentieth to similar debates about school discipline and the right of teachers to strike kids. Today, 19 states permit school officials to use corporeal punishment, and the controversy around school discipline has been heating up as discussions about local school governance have come up against federal education mandates. Legislators have conveniently divided along party lines to enter the fray. In Kansas, legislation that would allow teachers to use more forceful corporeal punishment has been proposed (but recently rejected), while in Texas a recent law allows parents to place their child on a “no-paddle” list, a position which allows spanking to remain in schools while granting certain parents (read: godless liberals) the ability to opt out of the paddle. The issue is not restricted to the United States. In Sweden, a furious debate has erupted over the calls by David Eberhard, a psychiatrist and former chief ER physician, to reclaim authoritarian parenting as a way to toughen up kids and prevent them from becoming “brats,” and in certain churches around the world, spanking does more than just toughen a kid up; it ensures they move on the path of righteousness. Spanking is biblical mandate, and it insinuates itself into the politics of parenting.
Those politics extend into the parenting publishing industry. Over the last three decades, most of the more popular books on discipline have focused on some permutation of teaching consequences, a residue of Skinner’s psychology that highlighted the observable facts of behavior at the expense of unobservable qualities such as emotions or the mind’s mysterious associations. The behaviorists envision humans as a collection of experiences that can be observed, documented, and manipulated, so that the goal of psychology and human improvement focuses on those external behaviors. Human motivations and intentions are, in this formulation, largely irrelevant. Who cares what you’re thinking when you decide to ignore that cigarette tempting you to smoke? All that matters is not picking it up. Wear a rubber band on your wrist and snap it, or chew on piece of gum, or go out with a friend, anything—your job is to worry less about what you’re thinking and feeling and focus on the behavioral consequences that will encourage you not to pick up the cigarette and take a draw.
The theory when applied to children suggests that teaching “logical” consequences to their actions will help them internalize the lesson on a kind of primal level. Spanking is the obvious example, but perhaps the most widely known consequence discipline method is one that reaches across political boundaries and seems to work for many regardless of socio-economic conditions. Developed in the mid-’80s by Dr. Richard Ferber, the director of the Center for Pediatric Sleep Disorders in Boston, the “cry-it-out” method teaches children to go to go to sleep on their own, and it has become a right of passage for many parents.
This question, more than any other these days, seems to mark us as either successful or unsuccessful parents. Good parents somehow are able to make their children sleep through the night, whereas the rest of us struggle with finding a good bed time, “putting the child down” (a phrase that reveals our desperation), and ensuring uninterrupted, ongoing, blissful, and quiet sleep. Those good parents, not including the self-righteous perfect-genes-perfect-babies ones who smile condescendingly at us mere mortals, may at some point decide that “cry-it-out” is the path to peace in our households.
The method works for many. With ruthless efficiency. If, by eight months or so, your child has not learned to go to sleep on her own, your job as a parent is to, through a process of gradual denial, remove yourself from her presence. Three or four nights after starting the process (perhaps less for those of you with wunderkinds), the wee-one learns that the crying results in absolutely nothing but her own exhaustion. Cry all she wants, mommy and daddy aren’t coming, so she might as well pipe down and rustle off to sleep. “Self-soothe” as the literature often calls it. Voila! Sleeping baby, sleeping parents. Playground bragging rights secured.
The problem is that this type of “discipline” has consequences that are largely invisible to us, but no less real than the silence in the house. The child does sleep—I can happily testify to that—but at what cost? The biological and neurological effects of cry-it-out are visible only when the brain is unmasked by technology that peers into brain functioning and bears witness to the complex ways our own chemistry responds to stress and to nurturing. That technology provides rather stark appraisals of what we do when we discipline in ways like allowing children to cry it out in order to find the consequence of falling asleep on their own, and while only some on the new frontier of discipline suggest that consequence methods in the way that we understand them today are ineffective, a growing chorus of researchers question the long-term effects of such methods. Short-term effectiveness, for them, is not the sole measure of desirability of a method of discipline.
The science of sleep has mixed responses to whether or not the method causes any sort of long-term, measurable problems. While the research agrees that “Ferberization” results in a flood of cortisol, a powerful stress hormone, over the brain, the full effects of that hormone bath are far from certain. Some, such as Dr. Bill Sears, whom Time magazine once labeled “the man who remade motherhood,” argue that such a wash over the brain may lead to unforeseen emotional and biological issues (including poor health as the child ages), while others argue that sleep is so important that the consequence of a three- or four- night, cry-it-out cortisol spike is minimal in comparison to the ongoing stress caused by poor sleep, which has been linked to any manner of issues that strike fear in the hearts of parents: poor academic performance, disobedience, lethargy, the Victorian brain fever, and downright nastiness.
Still, the fact of such a tremendous wash of stress hormone probably should not be dismissed. One might imagine, for example, a person experiencing a single, traumatic event. That event creates a similar hormone spike that can have important lasting effects on the brain and a person’s behavior. Because the mind, in all of its wisdom, associates that trauma with a variety of bodily sensations, the trauma can be re-activated by even the most innocuous happenstances. Picture here the veteran who jumps at the sound of a car backfiring. The brain of the veteran may experience the same rush of stress hormone as it did in the original event, so that the neurons reaffirm and build upon the effects of it: more stress, more hormone, more trauma. Further, the issue of whether or not a particular event causes stress hormone to be released is likely less significant than how the mind understands the relationship of that event to the individual. Questions like who was involved in the event may have more significance than simply the presence of the hormone alone because it indicates which parts of the brain will be involved in processing the stress. In the case of children, the stress initiated by a caregiver may be more significant in terms of brain neuroscience than the stress associated with, say, little Timmy’s school-yard friend Ginny, who knocks him off the swing set from time to time. That stress may cause the boy some difficulty, but the stress associated with an attachment figure leaving him at night to cry alone in his crib may be more significant. The child’s brain can only process that as an abandonment—it has no other way to make sense of it—and while the results of that abandonment vary considerably in any given household and certainly don’t sentence the child to a lifetime of despondency or, worse, mediocrity, the child’s brain experiences a lesson it simply cannot order or regulate except by associating care with something other than the parent. When using cry-it-out, then, parents not only teach sleep, they also teach the associations the child’s brain makes in order to help her feel soothed. Siegel and Bryson suggest thoughtful and engaged consideration of what those associations may be because they are influenced by our own behaviors. In thinking about consequence-based discipline, in other words, we need to consider the systems of self-regulation that we teach in subtle ways throughout each day. Some are likely not what we intend or desire.
Of course, most of us are about as likely to implement a daily ritual of “time-ins” as we are to finally begin that morning routine of sit-ups, push-ups, and chia-shakes. It’s not going to happen, and Siegel, I think, knows it. Siegel’s Brainstorm, which rushed to bestseller status last spring in advance of this fall’s co-authored book No-Drama Discipline, aimed to provide more realistic tools for parenting, especially for teenagers. No-Drama Discipline extends that to younger ages in order to suggest an entire re-orientation toward discipline. He wants to make time-in and its various permutations seem less of an event or “consequence” and more part of our daily routines. Instead of imagining a time-in as an isolated moment in the way that time-outs are, think of it instead as ongoing communication and building awareness.
For instance, Siegel suggests that after something noteworthy occurs, whether good or bad, you help your child tell the story of it. When I spoke with him, he described it like this: one day you go to the zoo, and an orangutan throws a banana at you and your kids. For a moment, you’re startled, but then you start to laugh, and you go on with your day. Siegel wants you to attend to the fact that when that banana came through the fence, you experienced a genuine and momentarily intense emotional response. As innocuous as it likely was, it occurred, and the odds are your child experienced it too. On your drive home, then, you bring that moment back from memory and tell a story about it: “Hey Timmy, remember when that orangutan threw that banana at your head? We were walking up to its cage, and we leaned really close it, and then he threw it. Oh my gosh, that scared me. It caught me by surprise and I couldn’t believe it happened. I was so surprised my heart jumped.” As many parents know, children will often step in with their own version to extend the story, and at that point, our job as parents is to acknowledge not only the event, but each person’s experience of the event. Even if the child, especially a young child, can’t name the feelings associated with it, we can, and we help them when we do. We put words to the intensity of the feelings, and we, accordingly, make them less mysterious and less intense. We, in other words, share our feelings in simple ways while helping the child understand his. He might say, “Yeah, and he made all those gross noises and shook his arms and that banana came right at my face.” And we say, knowing that besides the external, observable facts of the experience, an internal event was occurring in the child: “I can see how that scared you.” By teaching in low-stakes moments, our task as disciplinarians becomes easier in high stakes moments; our systems are better integrated.
For children, intense emotions are like a dark forest at night. Trees rustle in the wind, bats circle above, and all manner of insects crawl along the ground, but in the darkness they are almost impossible to see, let alone understand. The brain starts making associations, and the child becomes overwhelmed with dark imaginings. When we use discipline methods like time-out, we essentially usher our kids into the woods and just leave them there in the darkness. More, we actually tell them to sit there silently and not to move no matter what they experience so that they can “reflect” on their actions. Even as all the mysterious night sounds crunch and swirl around them, they must, we insist, ignore the dark noises and reflect on their behavior.
Most children do not take such a journey into the woods without putting up a fight, and few are likely capable of reflection when the screech owl beckons. Siegel attempts to make that journey less frightening and less rigid, possibly even inviting. Time-in encourages kids and parents to embark on a daytime adventure into the woods together and asks them to explore a little bit, to poke around in trees and piles of leaves to see what they can find. Parents hand kids tools to dig up dirt and tie branches together. They help them identify what they see and experience. They talk to them about what the woods look like in the dark and what other critters might come out besides squirrels and chipmunks. They may even make a hooting noise so that the kids have some sense of what night sounds like. The hope is that by getting to know the woods during the day, they aren’t quite so indecipherable at night, even if they are still dark and bit frightening.
This type of discipline requires us as parents to attend to not only the external reality of the event, but the internal and very personal reality as well. Intense emotions are in fact extraordinarily mysterious and frightening, even for adults. When we take just a moment to talk about how we experience them, we help children listen to that inner experience. We train them to recognize and adjust to them. When we do that, we provide them with access to feelings in an unassuming way, and we help them develop clearer visions of their own inner lives.
Discipline, in this model, transforms from punishment and obedience to teaching and self-regulation. Obedience may in certain instances be a by-product of teaching, but discipline means understanding child and brain development and building skills as children develop. Bryson recommends asking yourself two questions in any moment that you intend to “discipline” your child: what is it that I am wanting my child to learn at this moment, and what is the best way I can teach it? By teach, she means, quite literally, teach. Not insist, not demand, not coerce, not bribe. Teach. Instead of saying, “she needs to learn to take accountability for her actions,” we might say something like, “how can I best teach her to take better care of her bicycle?” Or instead of my thinking when my son throws his vegetables at me, “he needs to show some self-control,” I might be better served to ask myself, “how can I teach him what he’s allowed to throw and what he’s not allowed to throw.” When we consider what we want to teach and what skills we want to build, we develop more helpful interventions into the behaviors we want to limit. The concept is more difficult than it seems, and as an educator myself, I find it hard enough in the classroom, let alone in my house. It takes creativity, thoughtfulness, and, perhaps more than anything else, genuine attention to the situation and the child.
This neuropsychological approach that attends to inner lives to foster external behaviors does not provide magical bullets, but it isn’t aiming for that. It aims for a re-orientation from simple dictates (“he must stop doing that”) to nuanced teaching (“How can I teach him?”) in order to create (and this is the key) more durable changes in behavior. While spanking and “teaching consequences” often aim for immediate compliance, we shouldn’t fool ourselves that they result in that. I challenge you to show me the child whose whining ends after a swift hand to the ass. The crying typically escalates, followed quickly by, from the mouth of a seething parent, some permutation of the legendary phrase, “If you don’t quiet down, I’ll give you something to cry about.”
For those in interpersonal neurobiology, facilitating change is less about enforcing mandates and more about fostering new ways of being and living. Yes, they say, if a child reaches for a hot oven, we must act quickly and decisively, but discipline-as-teaching requires that we take that action and teach with it. In the case of the hot oven or a child darting into the street, we intercede swiftly, but we also demonstrate why it’s so dangerous and, the tough part, own up to our own fear and anxiety at that moment. We help the child see how we experienced it so that she understands why we acted in the way that we did. She in turn learns the name for the feelings inside of her when we yank her off the pavement. Yes, I could slap the child’s bum and just say “NO!,” and when I do, the child knows to not dart into the street again right away. But, I have also taught her that my fear for her safety results in her experiencing pain from me, a lesson that is the exact opposite of the one I really want to teach—which is that my fear for her safety means I will protect her at all costs. Showing your child that you are scared when she darts into the street—and calling it by its name—means that you are choosing to live with her in that moment. Both of your neurological systems are on high alert, and sharing that experience and recognizing it in yourself and in her creates trust that becomes wired in her nervous system. The child learns that her experience has been shared, that it can be described, and that it can be managed. She also learns, in ways that can never be taught by lecture or reprimand, what it means to respect another person’s experience because we have, in fact, shown respect for hers.[/ltr]
[ltr]Written by
Roger ThompsonAssociate professor, Stony Brook University
December 18, 2014[/ltr]
We're really messing with their brains. (AP Photo)
[ltr]At the end of a gravel road in the Chippewa National Forest of northern Minnesota, a group of camp counselors has gathered to hear psychotherapist Tina Bryson speak about neuroscience, mentorship, and camping. She is in Minnesota by invitation of the camp. Chippewa is at the front of a movement to bring brain science to bear on the camping industry; she keynoted this past year’s American Camping Association annual conference. As Bryson speaks to the counselors gathered for training, she emphasizes one core message: at the heart of effective discipline is curiosity—curiosity on the part of the counselors to genuinely understand and respect the campers’ experience while away from home.
Brain science is far from a precise field, but Bryson deploys it effectively when she conducts trainings. She has lectured from Australia to Germany, California to DC, and the camp trainings are only a small portion of what she does. She envisions herself as on a mission to change parenting, and her talks weave recent data on brain imaging, new findings from top journals, and reports of ongoing experimental research with stories from her own life, anecdotes from her clinical practice, and aw-shucks pithy sayings that help make the science accessible.
Bryson is not alone in this approach. She is part of a progressive new group of scientists, doctors, and psychologists whose goal is ambitious, if not outright audacious: they want to redefine “discipline” in order to change our culture. They want to rewrite, or perhaps more precisely said, rewire how we approach interacting with kids, and they want us to understand that our decisions about parenting affect not only our children’s minds, but ours as well.
So, we’re going to need to toss out our old discipline mainstays. Say goodbye to timeouts. So long spanking and other ritualized whacks. And cry-it-out sleep routines? Mercifully, they too can be a thing of the past. And yet, we can still help our children mature and grow. In fact, people like Bryson think we’ll do it better. If we are going to take seriously what science tells us about how we form relationships and how our mind develops, we will need to construct new strategies for parenting, and when we do, says this new group of researchers, we just may change the world.
How a child’s brain works
In the late 1980s, a group of scientists in Italy began work that would lead to a watershed discovery in 1992. Professor Giacomo Rizzolatti, a neuroscientist and MD, had been working with a research team at the University of Parma to understand the relationship between intent and motor function. How does our intention to do something result in us actually doing it? They conducted experiments on macaque monkeys, a genus of primates that includes some 20 species. Macaques were used in Harry Harlow’s infamous “Pit of Despair” experiments in the 1970s, and they have been subjected to experimentation to understand diseases as wide ranging as Parkinson’s to eye degeneration, from HIV to, most recently, the Ebola virus.In the early 1990s, Rizzolatti and his team had been conducting a series of experiments involving the monkeys and peanuts. A portion of the monkeys’ skulls had been removed, and the scientists had connected a series of wires directly to the brain. The wires conveyed electrical currents from the brain to sophisticated recording devices that could read the neurological patterns of the monkey’s actions and, presumably, its thinking. The experiment proceeded as expected until one day one of Rizollati’s team members noticed something unusual. When a monkey witnessed a researcher reaching for a peanut, the neurons in the monkey’s brain fired in precisely the same way that they did when the monkey itself reached for a peanut. In other words, as far as the brain was concerned, simply seeing someone grab a peanut is the same as actually grabbing it oneself.
The group immediately perceived the importance of the discovery. If seeing was, in terms of the brain, the same as doing, the entire canon of human behavior would have to be rewritten. When Rizolatti attempted to publish their findings, however, the first journal purportedly rejected the article because it found the results to be too inconsequential to consider, even if it did find them credible and accurate. A second journal, though, published details of the experiment, and several years later, another journal published a follow up set of findings. In that paper, Rizzolatti and his team coined a term for the neurological phenomenon they were witnessing: mirror neurons. The term would make him famous.
Since that time, hundreds if not thousands of articles have been published on mirror neurons. They have been credited with generating empathy in humans, fostering love between people, and providing new hope in the research on autism. Yet, the term and the idea of “mirror neurons” continue to prompt considerable controversy. Some researchers argue that empirical evidence for the existence of any neurons that function as “mirrors” is scant, while others suggest that neuroscience has yet to fully grasp the implications of neurons behaving in this way. Regardless of the final direction of these debates, the discussion about mirror neurons has pressed neuroscience into new frontiers, and it has suggested new avenues of inquiry for not only scientists, but doctors and psychologists. Among those avenues is a relatively recent field of study called interpersonal neurobiology. While mirror neurons are not the explicit foundation of the new field, the growth of it is virtually unimaginable without the discoveries by Rizzolatti and his Parma team.
Dan Siegel is the forefather of this movement, coining the term “interpersonal neurobiology” in the late 1990s and effectively launching the new discipline. His colleague Allan Schore, a neuropsychologist who now edits a Norton book series in the field, shares credit as co-founder of the field, but Siegel’s work has transitioned out of the research lab and into the popular press, and he has become an increasingly visible figure in the press circuit. A professor at UCLA’s School of Medicine and author of a flurry of bestsellers, Siegel has sent dozens of followers into the field over the last decade. Some, like Julia Wright and Tina Bryson, are finding their own footing as international experts and sought-after speakers, and they reflect the expanding influence of interpersonal neurobiology as a way of thinking about family relationships. Wright’s forthcoming Penguin release, Happy Sleeper (with Heather Turgeon), suggests new approaches to childhood sleep routines, and Bryson, Siegel’s co-author on the New York Times bestsellers The Whole-Brain Child and No Drama Discipline, redefines discipline more broadly in an attempt to encourage parents to reconsider their approach to child development.
Siegel remains a sought-after speaker in his own right, and the ideas emerging from his Mindful Awareness Research Center at UCLA are finding their ways into not only parenting networks and the popular press, but schools across the country. Some, like the Blue School in New York City, even shape their entire curriculum around his ideas.
Siegel received his medical degree from Harvard, and his contributions to science come in large part from his creation of the new, interdisciplinary field. Interpersonal neurobiology envisions the brain as a social organ, one whose processes can best be understood by its interaction with a variety of complex systems. Among those systems, and one that sets the field apart from simply neurology or biology, is the emotional system that develops in relationships. In terms of what happens inside the brain, that system is just as real as, say, the system that processes our eyesight and deciphers it into meaningful images for us.
Siegel, Bryson, and others in the field simplify the science behind their work by relying on the longstanding theory of the “triune brain,” a theory that divides the brain into three general parts: the brain stem, the limbic system, and the cerebral cortex. The cerebral cortex is our rational, human brain, and the brain stem is, in the words of many scientists, our “reptilian” or lizard brain, the one that basically exists to keep us alive in times of threat. The limbic system functions as a connector, but remains primitive, often encouraging behaviors without giving the cortex time to process and encourage a different, frequently better course of action. When we discipline, argue Siegel and Bryson, we often meet reactive, emotional limbic with limbic, or worse, the lizard brains take control in not just our child, who is raising hell and biting and hissing like a pissed off gecko, but in us as well, as we raise our voices and flail about trying to scare off the lizard by transforming into a bigger, meaner one. Will the Komodo dragon beat the gecko? In some ways, sure. But that little lizard learns one thing, and that is for it to win, it needs to grow stronger, get bigger, and bite harder. If, however, parents can channel their inner Steve Irwins and find ways to approach the lizard child with respect for how it is acting—which is ultimately in an adaptive and useful way to keep it alive in the face of danger and stress—then we might not only make contact with the creature, but teach it that it has nothing to fear so that it can back away, return to its cave, and let the less hissy, more rational kid come back out to play.
Our brain nurtures interaction with others through an almost impossibly complicated set of activities that can nonetheless be observed, documented, and, to some extent, measured. Relationships forge and develop neurons and neural pathways, which means that close observation of what actually occurs neurologically in the brain can help us understand what kinds of relationships work well and which do not. Equally important, the “brain” extends well beyond the organ within our skull. Nerves spread throughout the body, and those nerves are part of a larger body-wide brain that, for lack of a better term, wires together our thinking, our feelings, and our senses. They ultimately, as well, help us make sense of those very systems because we are capable of turning inward and becoming to some degree aware of the processes that make them work. This turning inward to observe and intentionally notice our own body and brain processes Siegel calls “mindsight,” and it allows individuals to make sense of their experiences and consciously integrate their various systems. Just as our five senses collect information that we use to discern the world around us, our mindsight can provide us with clearer views of our own internal world so that we can better relate to the people around us. When we see the mind as an intersecting web of systems whose processes, even if not fully understood, can be illuminated, then we have an opportunity to delve deeper into the significance of our relationships.
Much of what Siegel wants us to consider can be condensed into a simple phrase: “what fires together, wires together.” The idea is that when a set of neurons are stimulated, they link up with all those other neurons that are simultaneously firing. Whether the groups of neurons that are linking make sense to us as observers on the outside is beside the point. Odd pairings can occur, strange juxtapositions of feelings and sensations that, outside of the experience of a particular individual, seem almost impossible to the rest of us. I’m reminded of a narrative in the old DSM-IV casebook that describes an individual who had come to associate sexual arousal with being covered in insects. As a child, that individual had been locked into closets for unimaginable amounts of time, and during those times, bugs would frequently fill the space and crawl on him. The child, trying to seek some sort of escape from the reality of his experience, found comfort only in sexual release, even though he was too young to even know what sex was or meant. His body knew only that it felt good, and it provided the only possible escape available to him. It soothed in the midst of trauma. Those associations—comfort through sex and the sheer, incomprehensible horror, fear, and rage at being locked away in a closet full of insects—became in that mind, quite literally, wired together, so that sex, horror, pleasure, rage, and insects, became bundled as a mass of neurons that shared the same communication pathway. Siegel wants us to become aware of those types of associations and, just as importantly, types that are more mundane and quotidian. They are as much physical as they are “mental,” and they can be anything from unsurprising to astonishing. Sexual gratification and bugs, it turns out, can go together, despite what most of us imagine.
Despite their potential horror, these surprising pairings illustrate the complexity of the brain, and they suggest that if problematic associations can be wired together, so too can more uplifting ones. They point to the possibility that we can forge positive associations in order to create more meaningful, more encouraging, and more beautiful change when we discipline our children. We have known, though not always why, that if we can help a child associate certain behaviors and ways of being with positive stimuli, the child will likely want to replicate the behavior. Siegel and his followers argue that we will be less effective in helping children grow until we understand that children’s actions often have odd pairings at the heart of them and our role is to help children makes sense of them and, with any luck, shift them.
Spanking, attachment, and the brain
Each generation, of course, has its own child-rearing prophet, complete with magical gospels, and Siegel may be just another. Long before Siegel and his crew, discipline had been the foundation of many child experts’ careers. Dr. Benjamin Spock’s name remains virtually synonymous with infant and toddler child care, and Fritz Redl and William Wattenberg proposed a system of classroom discipline, the “pleasure pain principle,” that had its roots in the work of B. F. Skinner. Their work continues to shape education despite being theorized in the 1950s. In the nineteenth century, conduct manuals were bestsellers in America. They typically preached the need to limit undue stimulation of children lest they become perverted or uncivil, and as the century advanced, they increasingly associated discipline with corporeal punishment, to the degree that by the end of the century (as Jacob Middleton has suggested), parents and educators were rebelling. Some educators pointed out that while prisons (sometimes called “disciplinary barracks”) were moving away from physical punishment, schools were codifying it to allow its systematic use. Discipline became increasingly linked to educational debates, and that linkage gained momentum with the rise of behavioralism in the early and mid-twentieth century. The legacy has remained. In the mid-1990s, President Clinton demanded that “more discipline” be enacted at schools to combat the image of inner city schools as war zones, and in the wind up to his first term in office, President George W. Bush argued for legislation that would limit legal culpability of teachers using more stringent “discipline” in their classrooms.Today, the classroom remains the center of much debate about discipline, but it will likely be in the home that Siegel’s and Bryson’s ideas are put to the test. Their discussions of what happens in the brain when children are being “disciplined” in various ways conflict directly with certain of our parenting practices. The most telling point of conflict is one of the vexing questions of parenting that lingers from generation to generation: to spank or not to spank.
Among some quarters of the American populace, spanking is tantamount to abuse, yet in others, it remains a required part child rearing. The division between the two camps is apparent, as some researchers have suggested, along racial and, probably more tellingly, socio-economic lines. Recent discussions in response to allegations of abuse against Adrian Peterson bear witness to those lines—opportunistic punditry from the likes of Charles Barkley and Steve Harvey have connected the act of spanking to the cultural and economic realities of the black community.
Still, the division can also be drawn, if imperfectly, geographically. In the South, a growing movement over the last two decades has returned the rod to parenting. The movement, often couched in terms of “parental rights,” seeks to ensure that parents have the right to use corporeal punishment with their children. The difference is that the new spank world is not the swack-in-an-instant belting of our parents’ generation. This one is based on the idea that genuine love can be conveyed with a whack on the tush, and that children, because of their little lizard brains, understand and respond to physical stimuli better than verbal.
In fact, they are at least in part right. The animal brain of the child is quite sensitive to touch of all sorts. It recognizes the safety of a hug as well as the danger of a slap without the slightest bit of explanation, and it learns rather quickly that certain behaviors can lead to danger and a red ass. The problem, for folks like Siegel and Bryson, is that children enter a world of emotional chaos when their attachment figure, from whom they are wired to seek safety and security, becomes the figure who also inflicts physical harm. The animal brain, the one that seeks fight or flight, is at that moment conflicted, confused, and, probably downright pissed off. As the brain stem and limbic system instinctually tells the child that danger is coming and that he needs to seek safety and security in the embrace of the attachment figure, the limbic system also confronts the reality that the attachment figure is, in fact, the source of danger. Safety and danger conflated. Brain chemistry roundly fucked up. You might picture Curly running around in circles looking for a place to find safety from Moe, who pops him repeatedly on the forehead. Like poor Curly, lizard brain has very few ways to decipher what is happening and so just circles round and round, occasionally slapping anything or anyone (presumably Larry) nearby.
This internal conflict can lead to what neuropsychologists call “dysregulation.” The neurons start forging relationships that don’t make sense to the more advanced parts of our brain, and as the mind tries to integrate the information, it seeks out solutions, associations, and meaning. When that meaning is hard to discern, as in the case of spanking or even the threat of spanking, the child brain becomes increasingly frustrated, and it essentially dis-integrates. Melt-down ensues. More importantly, it is difficult to construct a coherent meaningful lesson and skills have not been built to make the child more adaptive next time. It has gained no new ways to interpret information, nor has it gained any new ways of making sense of the world with the cortex part of the brain. Instead, the brain-stem and the limbic area stay in charge, and the child, unable to process the conflict, learns a temporary, if also momentarily effective, lesson: if I do X, dad whips my tail. This is not insight or learning or skill-building, this is lizard logic.
Decades of research into spanking demonstrates what happens in the brain when we hit a child, even though it’s only been recently that we’ve been able to make sense of some of the findings. Children who are spanked are, according to a preponderance of the studies, more likely to commit crimes, more likely to suffer from depression, more likely to go to jail, more likely to get into fights, more likely to commit suicide, and more likely to abuse alcohol and drugs. They also typically have lower IQs and poorer academic performance. The studies are, of course, mixed, and they suggest correlations, not causations. We might expect, for instance, that a child with other learning issues may also have more behavioral issues, and so connecting the two doesn’t necessarily point to spanking as a culprit as much as some underlying issue with a particular child or a community. Still, a mountain of evidence suggests that spanking does very little good, if any at all. Most of this we know when we care to slow down and remember our own responses to spankings. Ask yourself, when you were spanked, did you absolutely, never again take an extra cookie from the cookie jar? In all likelihood, you probably just developed ninja-like stealth. Further, the research suggests that spanking may be, and I offer this tidbit without comment, more likely to be connected to political orientation than other traits. Spankers and spankees trend toward Republicanism. That last bit—political conservatives being and spawning spankers—points to why changing methods of discipline is so difficult and why science faces an uphill battle in facilitating change. Upending years of habits is hard enough with family politics, but when the issue becomes entrenched in national politics, it becomes even more difficult.
Discipline has always been politicized. The nineteenth-century debates that focused on whether or not corporeal punishment was appropriate for children gave way in the mid-twentieth to similar debates about school discipline and the right of teachers to strike kids. Today, 19 states permit school officials to use corporeal punishment, and the controversy around school discipline has been heating up as discussions about local school governance have come up against federal education mandates. Legislators have conveniently divided along party lines to enter the fray. In Kansas, legislation that would allow teachers to use more forceful corporeal punishment has been proposed (but recently rejected), while in Texas a recent law allows parents to place their child on a “no-paddle” list, a position which allows spanking to remain in schools while granting certain parents (read: godless liberals) the ability to opt out of the paddle. The issue is not restricted to the United States. In Sweden, a furious debate has erupted over the calls by David Eberhard, a psychiatrist and former chief ER physician, to reclaim authoritarian parenting as a way to toughen up kids and prevent them from becoming “brats,” and in certain churches around the world, spanking does more than just toughen a kid up; it ensures they move on the path of righteousness. Spanking is biblical mandate, and it insinuates itself into the politics of parenting.
Those politics extend into the parenting publishing industry. Over the last three decades, most of the more popular books on discipline have focused on some permutation of teaching consequences, a residue of Skinner’s psychology that highlighted the observable facts of behavior at the expense of unobservable qualities such as emotions or the mind’s mysterious associations. The behaviorists envision humans as a collection of experiences that can be observed, documented, and manipulated, so that the goal of psychology and human improvement focuses on those external behaviors. Human motivations and intentions are, in this formulation, largely irrelevant. Who cares what you’re thinking when you decide to ignore that cigarette tempting you to smoke? All that matters is not picking it up. Wear a rubber band on your wrist and snap it, or chew on piece of gum, or go out with a friend, anything—your job is to worry less about what you’re thinking and feeling and focus on the behavioral consequences that will encourage you not to pick up the cigarette and take a draw.
The theory when applied to children suggests that teaching “logical” consequences to their actions will help them internalize the lesson on a kind of primal level. Spanking is the obvious example, but perhaps the most widely known consequence discipline method is one that reaches across political boundaries and seems to work for many regardless of socio-economic conditions. Developed in the mid-’80s by Dr. Richard Ferber, the director of the Center for Pediatric Sleep Disorders in Boston, the “cry-it-out” method teaches children to go to go to sleep on their own, and it has become a right of passage for many parents.
Sleep wars and cry-it-out
Wander around a toddler playground, and most parents will be talking about sleeping. As children leap around like monkeys and infants list in strollers, a bubble of drool gathering at the corners of their mouths, parents will be sharing sleep war-stories. They will not be debating the benefits of Montessori education or discussing the virtues of early childhood music lessons. They will certainly not be discussing the benefits of breast-feeding or the nature of spousal relationships after childbirth. Those are much too high minded or wrought with guilt. No, they will be talking about sleep, and the conversation centers around one, dreaded, judgment-laden query: “is your kid making it through the night?”This question, more than any other these days, seems to mark us as either successful or unsuccessful parents. Good parents somehow are able to make their children sleep through the night, whereas the rest of us struggle with finding a good bed time, “putting the child down” (a phrase that reveals our desperation), and ensuring uninterrupted, ongoing, blissful, and quiet sleep. Those good parents, not including the self-righteous perfect-genes-perfect-babies ones who smile condescendingly at us mere mortals, may at some point decide that “cry-it-out” is the path to peace in our households.
The method works for many. With ruthless efficiency. If, by eight months or so, your child has not learned to go to sleep on her own, your job as a parent is to, through a process of gradual denial, remove yourself from her presence. Three or four nights after starting the process (perhaps less for those of you with wunderkinds), the wee-one learns that the crying results in absolutely nothing but her own exhaustion. Cry all she wants, mommy and daddy aren’t coming, so she might as well pipe down and rustle off to sleep. “Self-soothe” as the literature often calls it. Voila! Sleeping baby, sleeping parents. Playground bragging rights secured.
The problem is that this type of “discipline” has consequences that are largely invisible to us, but no less real than the silence in the house. The child does sleep—I can happily testify to that—but at what cost? The biological and neurological effects of cry-it-out are visible only when the brain is unmasked by technology that peers into brain functioning and bears witness to the complex ways our own chemistry responds to stress and to nurturing. That technology provides rather stark appraisals of what we do when we discipline in ways like allowing children to cry it out in order to find the consequence of falling asleep on their own, and while only some on the new frontier of discipline suggest that consequence methods in the way that we understand them today are ineffective, a growing chorus of researchers question the long-term effects of such methods. Short-term effectiveness, for them, is not the sole measure of desirability of a method of discipline.
The science of sleep has mixed responses to whether or not the method causes any sort of long-term, measurable problems. While the research agrees that “Ferberization” results in a flood of cortisol, a powerful stress hormone, over the brain, the full effects of that hormone bath are far from certain. Some, such as Dr. Bill Sears, whom Time magazine once labeled “the man who remade motherhood,” argue that such a wash over the brain may lead to unforeseen emotional and biological issues (including poor health as the child ages), while others argue that sleep is so important that the consequence of a three- or four- night, cry-it-out cortisol spike is minimal in comparison to the ongoing stress caused by poor sleep, which has been linked to any manner of issues that strike fear in the hearts of parents: poor academic performance, disobedience, lethargy, the Victorian brain fever, and downright nastiness.
Still, the fact of such a tremendous wash of stress hormone probably should not be dismissed. One might imagine, for example, a person experiencing a single, traumatic event. That event creates a similar hormone spike that can have important lasting effects on the brain and a person’s behavior. Because the mind, in all of its wisdom, associates that trauma with a variety of bodily sensations, the trauma can be re-activated by even the most innocuous happenstances. Picture here the veteran who jumps at the sound of a car backfiring. The brain of the veteran may experience the same rush of stress hormone as it did in the original event, so that the neurons reaffirm and build upon the effects of it: more stress, more hormone, more trauma. Further, the issue of whether or not a particular event causes stress hormone to be released is likely less significant than how the mind understands the relationship of that event to the individual. Questions like who was involved in the event may have more significance than simply the presence of the hormone alone because it indicates which parts of the brain will be involved in processing the stress. In the case of children, the stress initiated by a caregiver may be more significant in terms of brain neuroscience than the stress associated with, say, little Timmy’s school-yard friend Ginny, who knocks him off the swing set from time to time. That stress may cause the boy some difficulty, but the stress associated with an attachment figure leaving him at night to cry alone in his crib may be more significant. The child’s brain can only process that as an abandonment—it has no other way to make sense of it—and while the results of that abandonment vary considerably in any given household and certainly don’t sentence the child to a lifetime of despondency or, worse, mediocrity, the child’s brain experiences a lesson it simply cannot order or regulate except by associating care with something other than the parent. When using cry-it-out, then, parents not only teach sleep, they also teach the associations the child’s brain makes in order to help her feel soothed. Siegel and Bryson suggest thoughtful and engaged consideration of what those associations may be because they are influenced by our own behaviors. In thinking about consequence-based discipline, in other words, we need to consider the systems of self-regulation that we teach in subtle ways throughout each day. Some are likely not what we intend or desire.
The end of timeout
Siegel has suggested several methods of integrating the various neurological systems that comprise the “whole brain,” that web of neurons that extend from the brain in our head to the nervous systems distributed throughout our bodies. In terms of discipline, one of the most important is “time-in.” The idea of time-in is that parents direct attention to emotions to help children become aware of their inner lives. Teaching time-in means teaching mindfulness, but in order to prevent his idea of mindfulness from being conflated with teachings within religious traditions, Siegel coined the word “time-in.” While not developed as a counterpoint to time-outs—timeouts ideally foster reflectiveness—the term time-in aims for more consistent and ongoing engagement with emotions, communication, and relationships. Whereas timeout is typically a punishment (“You go sit there and think about what you’ve done!” hisses mom), time-in occurs throughout each day in order to subtly build awareness of the mind’s inner workings. With more awareness, the child has touchstones to which she can return in order to make sense of more intense emotional experiences—say when you refuse to buy yet another Skylander figure. Parents, likewise, can more intentionally guide children to recognize feelings when the stakes are a bit higher. Time-in, then, prevents escalation of bad behavior, because it helps a child learn to pay attention to the range of experiences he or she has on a daily basis.Of course, most of us are about as likely to implement a daily ritual of “time-ins” as we are to finally begin that morning routine of sit-ups, push-ups, and chia-shakes. It’s not going to happen, and Siegel, I think, knows it. Siegel’s Brainstorm, which rushed to bestseller status last spring in advance of this fall’s co-authored book No-Drama Discipline, aimed to provide more realistic tools for parenting, especially for teenagers. No-Drama Discipline extends that to younger ages in order to suggest an entire re-orientation toward discipline. He wants to make time-in and its various permutations seem less of an event or “consequence” and more part of our daily routines. Instead of imagining a time-in as an isolated moment in the way that time-outs are, think of it instead as ongoing communication and building awareness.
For instance, Siegel suggests that after something noteworthy occurs, whether good or bad, you help your child tell the story of it. When I spoke with him, he described it like this: one day you go to the zoo, and an orangutan throws a banana at you and your kids. For a moment, you’re startled, but then you start to laugh, and you go on with your day. Siegel wants you to attend to the fact that when that banana came through the fence, you experienced a genuine and momentarily intense emotional response. As innocuous as it likely was, it occurred, and the odds are your child experienced it too. On your drive home, then, you bring that moment back from memory and tell a story about it: “Hey Timmy, remember when that orangutan threw that banana at your head? We were walking up to its cage, and we leaned really close it, and then he threw it. Oh my gosh, that scared me. It caught me by surprise and I couldn’t believe it happened. I was so surprised my heart jumped.” As many parents know, children will often step in with their own version to extend the story, and at that point, our job as parents is to acknowledge not only the event, but each person’s experience of the event. Even if the child, especially a young child, can’t name the feelings associated with it, we can, and we help them when we do. We put words to the intensity of the feelings, and we, accordingly, make them less mysterious and less intense. We, in other words, share our feelings in simple ways while helping the child understand his. He might say, “Yeah, and he made all those gross noises and shook his arms and that banana came right at my face.” And we say, knowing that besides the external, observable facts of the experience, an internal event was occurring in the child: “I can see how that scared you.” By teaching in low-stakes moments, our task as disciplinarians becomes easier in high stakes moments; our systems are better integrated.
For children, intense emotions are like a dark forest at night. Trees rustle in the wind, bats circle above, and all manner of insects crawl along the ground, but in the darkness they are almost impossible to see, let alone understand. The brain starts making associations, and the child becomes overwhelmed with dark imaginings. When we use discipline methods like time-out, we essentially usher our kids into the woods and just leave them there in the darkness. More, we actually tell them to sit there silently and not to move no matter what they experience so that they can “reflect” on their actions. Even as all the mysterious night sounds crunch and swirl around them, they must, we insist, ignore the dark noises and reflect on their behavior.
Most children do not take such a journey into the woods without putting up a fight, and few are likely capable of reflection when the screech owl beckons. Siegel attempts to make that journey less frightening and less rigid, possibly even inviting. Time-in encourages kids and parents to embark on a daytime adventure into the woods together and asks them to explore a little bit, to poke around in trees and piles of leaves to see what they can find. Parents hand kids tools to dig up dirt and tie branches together. They help them identify what they see and experience. They talk to them about what the woods look like in the dark and what other critters might come out besides squirrels and chipmunks. They may even make a hooting noise so that the kids have some sense of what night sounds like. The hope is that by getting to know the woods during the day, they aren’t quite so indecipherable at night, even if they are still dark and bit frightening.
This type of discipline requires us as parents to attend to not only the external reality of the event, but the internal and very personal reality as well. Intense emotions are in fact extraordinarily mysterious and frightening, even for adults. When we take just a moment to talk about how we experience them, we help children listen to that inner experience. We train them to recognize and adjust to them. When we do that, we provide them with access to feelings in an unassuming way, and we help them develop clearer visions of their own inner lives.
Discipline, in this model, transforms from punishment and obedience to teaching and self-regulation. Obedience may in certain instances be a by-product of teaching, but discipline means understanding child and brain development and building skills as children develop. Bryson recommends asking yourself two questions in any moment that you intend to “discipline” your child: what is it that I am wanting my child to learn at this moment, and what is the best way I can teach it? By teach, she means, quite literally, teach. Not insist, not demand, not coerce, not bribe. Teach. Instead of saying, “she needs to learn to take accountability for her actions,” we might say something like, “how can I best teach her to take better care of her bicycle?” Or instead of my thinking when my son throws his vegetables at me, “he needs to show some self-control,” I might be better served to ask myself, “how can I teach him what he’s allowed to throw and what he’s not allowed to throw.” When we consider what we want to teach and what skills we want to build, we develop more helpful interventions into the behaviors we want to limit. The concept is more difficult than it seems, and as an educator myself, I find it hard enough in the classroom, let alone in my house. It takes creativity, thoughtfulness, and, perhaps more than anything else, genuine attention to the situation and the child.
This neuropsychological approach that attends to inner lives to foster external behaviors does not provide magical bullets, but it isn’t aiming for that. It aims for a re-orientation from simple dictates (“he must stop doing that”) to nuanced teaching (“How can I teach him?”) in order to create (and this is the key) more durable changes in behavior. While spanking and “teaching consequences” often aim for immediate compliance, we shouldn’t fool ourselves that they result in that. I challenge you to show me the child whose whining ends after a swift hand to the ass. The crying typically escalates, followed quickly by, from the mouth of a seething parent, some permutation of the legendary phrase, “If you don’t quiet down, I’ll give you something to cry about.”
For those in interpersonal neurobiology, facilitating change is less about enforcing mandates and more about fostering new ways of being and living. Yes, they say, if a child reaches for a hot oven, we must act quickly and decisively, but discipline-as-teaching requires that we take that action and teach with it. In the case of the hot oven or a child darting into the street, we intercede swiftly, but we also demonstrate why it’s so dangerous and, the tough part, own up to our own fear and anxiety at that moment. We help the child see how we experienced it so that she understands why we acted in the way that we did. She in turn learns the name for the feelings inside of her when we yank her off the pavement. Yes, I could slap the child’s bum and just say “NO!,” and when I do, the child knows to not dart into the street again right away. But, I have also taught her that my fear for her safety results in her experiencing pain from me, a lesson that is the exact opposite of the one I really want to teach—which is that my fear for her safety means I will protect her at all costs. Showing your child that you are scared when she darts into the street—and calling it by its name—means that you are choosing to live with her in that moment. Both of your neurological systems are on high alert, and sharing that experience and recognizing it in yourself and in her creates trust that becomes wired in her nervous system. The child learns that her experience has been shared, that it can be described, and that it can be managed. She also learns, in ways that can never be taught by lecture or reprimand, what it means to respect another person’s experience because we have, in fact, shown respect for hers.[/ltr]
Guest- Guest
Re: Hit children?
Ill rock my kids if need be. That way they learn gawd dammit!!!!
OldBlue- Made Member
- Number of posts : 762
Registration date : 2009-03-11
Location : Pit of HELL
Re: Hit children?
The moment my son is old enough, and he talks back, I'm whooping that ass like a step child.
Forum Gawd- Boss
- Number of posts : 4842
Registration date : 2009-09-16
Age : 30
Location : Athol Street Nd Hemlock.!!!
Re: Hit children?
You pieces of shit.
Well I suppose that's an Improvement, because there was this one study that came out saying a big percentage of mothers hit their babies in anger several times a week... Babies... That can't talk or walk yet.
Well I suppose that's an Improvement, because there was this one study that came out saying a big percentage of mothers hit their babies in anger several times a week... Babies... That can't talk or walk yet.
Re: Hit children?
My question is, if the child does something and they are hit, but continue that behavior, how is hitting thought to be an appropriate response?
Guest- Guest
Re: Hit children?
jennierock wrote:My question is, if the child does something and they are hit, but continue that behavior, how is hitting thought to be an appropriate response?
You hit them harder, But not babies, Babies are cute and shit.
OldBlue- Made Member
- Number of posts : 762
Registration date : 2009-03-11
Location : Pit of HELL
Re: Hit children?
They are cute and they do shit.OldBlue wrote:jennierock wrote:My question is, if the child does something and they are hit, but continue that behavior, how is hitting thought to be an appropriate response?
You hit them harder, But not babies, Babies are cute and shit.
Guest- Guest
Re: Hit children?
I almost beat my bitchs ass the other day for smacking my daughters hand just for pulling hair. The only reason I didn't is because I'm learning to control my anger on my loved ones. I never take it out on my daughter. Sometimes her constant crying irritates me, but that's just the life of a parent. I remember there being a piece of shit in jail for beating a 6 month old he was baby sitting brain dead just because he wouldn't stop crying. I hope he got the ass whooping, rape, and stabbings he deserves in there. He pc'd up before we found out about his charges, so we couldn't break his neck unfortunately.
Forum Gawd- Boss
- Number of posts : 4842
Registration date : 2009-09-16
Age : 30
Location : Athol Street Nd Hemlock.!!!
Re: Hit children?
That's good because a big part of whats wrong with the world is shitty parents who take out their anger on their kids.
I was reading comments where people were debating this and one argument put forth was that children are cannot reason like an adult so you must smack them anytime they do something that you don't like. Which I think is ridiculous . If anything, that would make hitting them even worse because it would be unfair to hit someone that doesn't have the mental capacity to reason.
Some of the arguments used in favor of hitting children were the same exact arguments used to justify husbands who hit their wives decades ago.
Hitting your wife is bad but not even as bad as hitting children because at least your wife is an adult with an ability to leave or divorce and she chose her husband, but kids don't chose their parents and are dependent on them.
Oh but my favorite line used by people in favor of whooping their kids is the classic " well I was whooped as a child and I turned out just fine" lol!
Ok you fuckin perfect human being.
I was reading comments where people were debating this and one argument put forth was that children are cannot reason like an adult so you must smack them anytime they do something that you don't like. Which I think is ridiculous . If anything, that would make hitting them even worse because it would be unfair to hit someone that doesn't have the mental capacity to reason.
Some of the arguments used in favor of hitting children were the same exact arguments used to justify husbands who hit their wives decades ago.
Hitting your wife is bad but not even as bad as hitting children because at least your wife is an adult with an ability to leave or divorce and she chose her husband, but kids don't chose their parents and are dependent on them.
Oh but my favorite line used by people in favor of whooping their kids is the classic " well I was whooped as a child and I turned out just fine" lol!
Ok you fuckin perfect human being.
Re: Hit children?
Anyone that says children can't reason either doesn't know children or doesn't pay attention to their children.
Guest- Guest
Re: Hit children?
Hitting a child with a closed fist or an object ie belt, stick, shoe IMO is flat out wrong...a slapped ass never killed anyone though, and sometimes gets the message across when repeated verbal chastisement doesn't seem to work...
Like the old time Jamaicans say 'those that don't hear must FEEL!'
Like the old time Jamaicans say 'those that don't hear must FEEL!'
chang- Soldier
- Number of posts : 183
Registration date : 2009-01-03
Page 1 of 1
Permissions in this forum:
You cannot reply to topics in this forum
Sat Oct 12, 2024 12:51 am by socalifascolonias
» Inland Empire Gang List
Fri Aug 02, 2024 4:12 am by kamikazi1
» Perris Street Gangs
Thu Jul 25, 2024 12:45 pm by Blakkkk
» SOUTHSIDE COLTON LA PALOMA PARK LOKOS Ost GANG
Mon Apr 08, 2024 9:42 pm by Esemuggzy
» MCP13 WHO ARE THEY?
Sun Feb 25, 2024 8:09 pm by villejuggin
» Gangs that have died out
Mon Jan 15, 2024 11:59 am by Morrolooooks
» Fontana pt2
Sun Jan 14, 2024 11:59 am by Morrolooooks
» Inactive Fontana gangs
Sat Jan 13, 2024 5:43 pm by Morrolooooks
» IE gangs in the 90s
Sat Jan 13, 2024 3:58 am by 627.loka